LitCharts assigns a color and icon to each theme in Thinking, Fast and Slow, which you can use to track the themes throughout the work.
Intuition, Deliberation, and Laziness
Human Fallibility and Overconfidence
Stories and Subjectivity vs. Statistics and Objectivity
Choices, Losses, and Gains
Summary
Analysis
Kahneman relays that System 2 has a natural speed. We expend some mental energy in considering random thoughts and in monitoring what goes on around us, but normally there is little strain. We make many small decisions and absorb pieces of information without much effort.
The normal effortlessness of our everyday mental processes contributes to our laziness in dealing with more complicated thoughts and calculations, as we work to exert as little effort as possible.
Active
Themes
Normally, it is easy to walk and think at the same time, but at the extremes these activities compete for resources. If we are asked while walking to compute 23 x 78, we will surely stop. Or if we walk at a very fast pace, we hinder our ability to think.
This concept becomes a bedrock principle of why we make mistakes: in addition to the fact that we are lazy thinkers by nature, we also often have to think about many things simultaneously, which hinders our ability to think about each concept with equal care.
Active
Themes
Often, we also have to monitor our self-control while we think. Maintaining difficult cognitive work requires effort not only in thinking, but in forcing ourselves to continue that work (like reading a difficult book). Sometimes, however, people are able to expend considerable effort for long periods of time without having to exert willpower, which psychologist Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi calls flow.
The concept of having to think hard and simultaneously having to exert willpower exposes why cognitive effort seems so difficult. Deliberation is inherently more difficult than relying on intuition.
Active
Themes
It has been proven that self-control and cognitive effort are both forms of mental work. People who are asked to retain seven digits for a minute or two and are simultaneously offered the choice between a fruit salad and chocolate cake are more likely to select the cake. When we are cognitively busy, we are less able to maintain self-control.
This concept provides another reason why we prefer to be lazy: cognitive effort not only effects the energy we exert with our minds, but also effects many other, unrelated choices as we become less able to avoid various temptations.
Psychologist Roy Baumeister discovered that if you have to force yourself to do something, you are less willing or less able to exert self-control when the next challenge presents itself, something called ego depletion. Or, if you successfully exert self-control in one task, you often do not feel like making an effort in another.
Cognitive laziness, then, is be particularly difficult to avoid; as we are forced to face it again and again, while we become more and more “ego depleted.”
Baumeister’s group also showed that the idea of “mental energy” is not a metaphor. The nervous system consumes more glucose than most other parts of the body, and people who were given lemonade with sugar before participating in a focusing task did not show the same ego depletion that people who were given lemonade with Splenda.
Kahneman explains how the concept of cognitive laziness is not just a psychological principle, but actually one rooted in cognitive science: the more glucose we consume, the less depleted we become. It is possible that this also explains our preference for the chocolate cake over the fruit salad in Kahneman’s earlier example.
A disturbing effect of ego depletion in judgment was recently reported. Judges in Israel spent days reviewing applications for parole. The judges were much more likely (65% approval vs. 35% approval) to approve requests at the beginning of the day and after a meal. When they are ego depleted, they fall back on the easier position of denying parole.
Even though the statistics Kahneman presents are staggering, one would hope that these statistics are then acknowledged by judges and that they are able to counter this effect so that meal breaks do not have an effect on the judges’ determinations.
One of the main functions of System 2 is to monitor the actions “suggested” by System 1. Kahneman provides a sample puzzle: “A bat and ball cost $1.10. The bat costs one dollar more than the ball. How much does the ball cost?” The intuitive answer is 10 cents, but this is the wrong answer. More than 50% of the students at Harvard, MIT, and Princeton gave the incorrect answer. At less selective universities, the rate of failure was 85%.
This puzzle is a quintessential example of how, even though we could put a little more effort into calculation and see that our intuitive answer is wrong, we stick with our intuitive answer because it comes up quickly and readily, and because we are inherently lazy.
These percentages are shocking, considering that checking the math would require only a few seconds of extra work. Failure to answer the bat-and-ball problem correctly, as well as other puzzles like it, is a matter of insufficient motivation. People who avoid the wrong answer are more alert, intellectually active, and more skeptical about their intuitions.
Like learning various visual illusions, the bat-and-ball problem represents a cognitive illusion that we have to learn not to trust. This also plays into overconfidence: those who are less willing to be confident in their answers will check their work and discover the correct answer.
Researchers have spent some time trying to discover the connection between cognitive aptitude and self-control. In a famous experiment, Walter Mischel gave four-year-old children the choice between a small reward (one Oreo) which they could receive at any time, or a larger reward (two cookies) for which they could wait 15 minutes.
Mischel’s test focuses on self-control as a predictor of aptitude. In line with Kahneman’s earlier arguments, theoretically those who forgo the cookie would be more “intellectually active” because they would exhibit self-control in this and in other aspects of their lives.
About half of the children manage to wait for the two cookies. Ten or fifteen years later, a large gap had opened up between those who could wait and those who could not. The resisters had high measures of self-control, were less likely to take drugs, and had substantially higher scores on intelligence tests.
In Mischel’s study, forgoing the cookie not only correlated to more self-control now and in the future, but it also tied to less laziness as they scored higher on intelligence and aptitude tests.
This test shows that some people think more with System 1, and some think more with System 2. Keith Stanovich and Richard West, who introduced the terms System 1 and System 2, believe that intelligence is not the only thing that distinguishes these two kinds of people. Stanovich believes they are more rational—a quality he believes to be distinct from intelligence.
It is particularly notable that Stanovich makes a distinction between intelligence and rationality, perhaps asserting that people who do well on cognitive aptitude tests are not necessarily more intelligent but are instead more willing to question and check their work.